Cover Page

  1. Methods and Principles in Medicinal Chemistry
  2. Edited by R. Mannhold, H. Kubinyi, G. Folkers Editorial Board:
  3. H. Buschmann, H. Timmerman, H. van de Waterbeemd, John Bondo Hansen

Previous Volumes of this Series:

  1. Holenz, Jörg (Ed.)
  2. Lead Generation
  3. Methods and Strategies
  4. 2016
  5. ISBN: 978-3-527-33329-5
    Vol.68
  1. Erlanson, Daniel A. / Jahnke, Wolfgang (Eds.)
  2. Fragment-based Drug Discovery
  3. Lessons and Outlook
  4. 2015
  5. ISBN: 978-3-527-33775-0
    Vol. 67
  1. Urbán, László / Patel, Vinod F. / Vaz, Roy J. (Eds.)
  2. Antitargets and Drug Safety
  3. 2015
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33511-4
    Vol. 66
  1. Keserü, György M. / Swinney, David C. (Eds.)
  2. Kinetics and Thermodynamics of Drug Binding
  3. 2015
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33582-4
    Vol. 65
  1. Pfannkuch, Friedlieb / Suter-Dick, Laura (Eds.)
  2. Predictive Toxicology
  3. From Vision to Reality
  4. 2014
  5. ISBN: 978-3-527-33608-1
    Vol. 64
  1. Kirchmair, Johannes (Ed.)
  2. Drug Metabolism Prediction
  3. 2014
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33566-4
    Vol. 63
  1. Vela, José Miguel / Maldonado, Rafael / Hamon, Michel (Eds.)
  2. In vivo Models for Drug Discovery
  3. 2014
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33328-8
    Vol. 62
  1. Liras, Spiros / Bell, Andrew S. (Eds.)
  2. Phosphodiesterases and Their Inhibitors
  3. 2014
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33219-9
    Vol. 61
  1. Hanessian, Stephen (Ed.)
  2. Natural Products in Medicinal Chemistry
  3. 2014
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33218-2
    Vol. 60
  1. Lackey, Karen / Roth, Bruce (Eds.)
  2. Medicinal Chemistry Approaches to Personalized Medicine
  3. 2013
  4. ISBN: 978-3-527-33394-3
    Vol. 59

Edited by Marianne I. Martic-Kehl and P. August Schubiger

Animal Models for Human Cancer

Discovery and Development of Novel Therapeutics

 

 

Wiley Logo

List of Contributors

  1. Carlos E. Alvarez
  2. The Ohio State University
  3. Medicine and Veterinary Medicine
  4. 700 Children's Drive, W431
  5. Columbus, OH 43205
  6. USA

 

  1. Karin Blumer
  2. Novartis International AG
  3. Fabrikstr. 6
  4. 4002 Basel
  5. Switzerland

 

  1. Guido Bocci
  2. University of Pisa
  3. Clinical and Experimental Medicine
  4. Via Roma 55
  5. 56126 Pisa
  6. Italy

 

  1. Gianluca Boo
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. ETH Zürich
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Lex M. Bouter
  2. VU University Medical Center
  3. Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics
  4. De Boelelaan 1117
  5. 1081 HV Amsterdam
  6. The Netherlands

 

  1. Urban Emmenegger
  2. University of Toronto
  3. Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre
  4. 2075 Bayview Avenue
  5. Toronto, ON M4N3M5
  6. Canada

 

  1. Joelle M. Fenger
  2. Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences
  3. The Ohio State University
  4. Veterinary Medical Center
  5. 601 Vernon Tharp Street
  6. Columbus, OH 43210
  7. USA

 

  1. Michael F.W. Festing
  2. University of Leicester
  3. MRC Toxicology Unit
  4. Lancaster Road
  5. Leicester LEI 9HN
  6. UK

 

  1. Giulio Francia
  2. The University of Texas at El Paso
  3. Department of Biological Sciences,
  4. Border Biomedical Research Center
  5. El Paso, TX 79902
  6. USA

 

  1. Ramona Graf
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. ETH Zürich
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Katrin Grüntzig
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. ETH Zürich
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Robert S. Kerbel
  2. University of Toronto
  3. Biological Sciences Platform
  4. Sunnybrook Research Institute
  5. S-217, 2075 Bayview Avenue
  6. Toronto, ON M4N 3M5
  7. Canada

 

  1. William C. Kisseberth
  2. The Ohio State University
  3. Department of Veterinary Clinical Sciences
  4. 448 VMAB
  5. 1900 Coffey Rd.
  6. Columbus, OH 43210
  7. USA

 

  1. Esther K. Lee
  2. University of Toronto
  3. Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre
  4. 2075 Bayview Avenue
  5. Toronto, ON M4N3M5
  6. Canada

 

  1. Cheryl A. London
  2. Department of Veterinary Biosciences
  3. The Ohio State University
  4. 454 VMAB
  5. 1900 Coffey Rd.
  6. Columbus, OH 43210
  7. USA

 

  1. Simone Macri
  2. Istituto Superiore di Sanità
  3. Department of Cell Biology and Neuroscience
  4. Viale Regina Elena 299
  5. 00161 Roma
  6. Italy

 

  1. Marianne I. Martic-Kehl
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. STW/ETH-Zentrum
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Irving Miramontes
  2. The University of Texas at El Paso
  3. Department of Biological Sciences
  4. Border Biomedical Research Center
  5. 500 W. University Avenue
  6. El Paso, TX 79902
  7. USA

 

  1. Anthony J. Mutsaers
  2. Department of Clinical Studies
  3. Department of Biomedical Sciences
  4. Ontario Veterinary College
  5. University of Guelph
  6. 50 Stone Road
  7. Guelph, ON N1G2W1
  8. Canada

 

  1. Karla Parra
  2. The University of Texas at El Paso
  3. Department of Biological Sciences
  4. Border Biomedical Research Center
  5. 500 W. University Avenue
  6. El Paso, TX 79902
  7. USA

 

  1. Andreas Pospischil
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. ETH Zürich
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Gerben ter Riet
  2. University of Amsterdam
  3. Academic Medical Center, J2-116
  4. Meibergdreef 9
  5. 1105 AZ Amsterdam
  6. The Netherlands

 

  1. S. Helene Richter
  2. University of Münster
  3. Department of Behavioural Biology
  4. Badestraße 13
  5. 48149 Münster
  6. Germany

 

  1. Jennie L. Rowell
  2. Center of Excellence in Critical and Complex Care
  3. College of Nursing
  4. The Ohio State University
  5. 390 Newton Hall
  6. 1585 Neil Ave.
  7. Columbus, OH 43210
  8. USA

 

  1. Chiara Spinello
  2. Istituto Superiore di Sanità
  3. Department of Cell Biology and Neuroscience
  4. Viale Regina Elena 299
  5. 00161 Roma
  6. Italy

 

  1. P. August Schubiger
  2. Collegium Helveticum
  3. ETH Zürich
  4. Schmelzbergstr. 25
  5. 8092 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Divya Vats
  2. ETH Zürich
  3. Institute for Biomedical Engineering
  4. Wolfgang-Pauli-Str. 27
  5. 8093 Zürich
  6. Switzerland

 

  1. Isain Zapata
  2. Center for Molecular and Human Genetics
  3. The Research Institute at Nationwide Children's Hospital
  4. 700 Children's Drive
  5. Columbus, OH 43205
  6. USA

Preface

The second demand of The Three R guiding principles for more ethical use of animals in testing (already established in 1959 [1]) reads:

Reduction: use of methods that enable researchers to obtain comparable levels of information from fewer animals, or to obtain more information from the same number of animals.

Quoted from Wikipedia July 2015

The present volume on Animal Models for Human Cancer: Discovery and Development of Novel Therapeutics by Marianne Martic-Kehl and P. August Schubiger focuses in essence on the design and numerical evaluation of animal tests in cancer drug development. This field of research needs special attention because of various reasons. The most challenging one being the quality and validity of human tumor model in rodents, the most frequent used animal in cancer drug development. Since mice do not develop spontaneous human cancer, genetically modified organisms are the standard. Higher animals, like cats and dogs, would be the choice then, implicating an ethical conflict that is also at a “higher” level, besides the cost.

The volume editors have been able to invite distinguished experts from leading institutions and research organizations to reason about the current situation, to analyze pros and cons, and to come up with new suggestions to improve the situation. While this is definitely difficult to do for the animal experiment itself, much neglect can be detected in its statistical evaluation. How can it be that good experimental practice is violated in so many papers that have all passed peer review. Randomization, multiple use of the animals, clear and validated endpoints, and sufficient numbers are very often omitted or not commented on, as a meta-study of several hundred of recent publications in the field has revealed. Here, the second demand of the 3Rs is affected to a considerable extent, and at the same time could easily be followed by a more rigorous peer review system.

Marianne Martic-Kehl and P. August Schubiger deserve deep respect for tackling problems, which have been of concern for several decennia, but have always been and still are a kind of taboo. You don't make friends in the scientific community by asking those nasty questions. Hence, the scientific community should be grateful for the researchers having the “guts” to point to neglect and suggest alternatives and improvements.

In addition, we are very much indebted to Frank Weinreich and Waltraud Wüst, both at Wiley-VCH. Their support and ongoing engagement, not only for this book but for the whole series Methods and Principles in Medicinal Chemistry adds to the success of this excellent collection of monographs on various topics, all related to drug research.

 

Düsseldorf

Weisenheim am Sand

Zürich

Raimund Mannhold
Hugo Kubinyi
Gerd Folkers

January 2016

Reference

  1. 1. Russell, W.M.S. and Burch, R.L. (1959). The Principles of Humane Experimental Technique, London: Methuen.

A Personal Foreword

As animal well being is a prerequisite for reliable experimental results, it is of utmost importance to seek for methods and procedures that can reduce suffering of the animals and improve their welfare.

This sentence, closing Vera Baumans' conclusion in a 2004 paper on ethical dilemmas in animal research [1], unfolds the dilemma of what suffering means for animals. What is the perspective to be taken, where to position the borderline between objectivity and subjectivity? Are anthropocentric views the good or the flipside of the coin?

While it is uncontested that animals feel pain, the question remains of which kind it is. The McGill pain questionnaire will not apply to rodents. This question has been around in the literature for almost 4000 years. Animals as “tools” for research are ascribed to the times of Hippocrates, which is still under dispute. The famous Roman physician Galenus, however, became known as the Father of Vivisection. The debate probably flared up for the first time in the seventeenth century. One of the founding fathers of the enlightenment, the Dutch philosopher Baruch de Spinoza, “admitted that animals suffer, but we are within our moral rights to use them, as we please, treating them in the way which best suits us; for their nature is not like ours, and their emotions are naturally different from human emotions” [2]. This view seems to be enforced by what is widely known as the “Cartesian Gap,” assuming that Descartes categorized animals as “meaty machines” or as automata. However, when it comes to emotions I get the impression that Descartes – at least for the human being – is somewhat conciliating: Ainsi que souuent vne mesme action, qui nous est agreable lors que nous sommes en bonne humeur, nous peut déplaire lors que nous sommes tristes & chagrins.1 It has been more than 100 years later that Jeremy Bentham fiercely and provokingly opposed the Cartesian perspective:

The French have already discovered that the blackness of the skin is no reason why a human being should be abandoned without redress to the caprice of a tormentor. It may one day come to be recognized that the number of the legs, the villosity of the skin, or the termination of the os sacrum [tailbone),are reasons equally insufficient for abandoning a sensitive being to the same fate. What else is it that should trace the insuperable line? Is it the faculty of reason, or perhaps the faculty of discourse? But a full grown horse or dog is beyond comparison a more rational as well as more conversable animal, than an infant of a day, or even, a month old. But suppose they were otherwise, what would it avail? The question is not, Can they reason? nor Can they talk? but Can they suffer? [3]

Bentham is regarded among the first to foster animal rights, the ability to suffer being the benchmark, the insuperable line, instead of the ability to reason. Again approximately after a century Darwin established the biological similarities between man and animal. Not surprisingly, however, his seminal scientific findings led to an increase in animal experimentation [1], since they paved the ground for a rationale to use animals as a model for human physiology and biological function.

It was Darwin's contemporary, the great physiologist Claude Bernard, who established this similarity between man and animal as a scientific method and became the founding father of modern experimental medicine [4]. His key message precisely describes the contemporary paradigm of biomedical research:

Le médecin qui est jaloux de mériter ce nom dans le sens scientifique doit, en sortant de l'hôpital, aller dans son laboratoire, et c'est là qu'il cherchera par des expériences sur les animaux à se rendre compte de ce qu'il a observé chez ses malades, soit relativement au mécanisme des maladies, soit relativement à l'action des médicaments, soit relativement à l'origine des lésions morbides des organes ou des tissus. C'est là, en un mot, qu'il fera la vraie science médicale.2

Claude Bernard incorporated the principles of “hard science,” in particular physics and chemistry, into the realm of medical research and made them the cornerstones of his scientific method [4]. Since (physical or chemical) experiments in humans are clearly beyond any moral or legal acceptance, animal “deputies” became the scientific object to serve as the mere substance in modeling a human disease.

With the advent of genetic modification techniques in the 1980s, transgenic animals and in particular rodents—mice being the working horses of modern biomedical animal experimentation—opened a new era in disease modeling. Single gene function, genetic components, and regulatory networks could be correlated with diseased conditions in humans. Still, we are facing the problem of “bridging the gap” in between mouse genomics and the disease phenomenon in the individual human being. Hence, the front edge research in biomedical is focusing on non-human primates (NHPs).

Due to physiologic differences between rodents and higher primates, such as life span, brain size and complexity and motor repertoire, as well as the availability of cognitive behavioral testing, NHPs are considered one of the best animal models; especially for complex disorders that correlate with aging, cognitive behavioral function, mental development, and psychiatric dysfunctions. In addition to neural psychiatric related disorders, metabolic function, reproductive physiology, and immunology are other areas of research where the NHP model has been widely used. [5]

Given the complexity of a disease or illness, the causality of which is, as we increasingly understand, far from a simple “one gene, one disease” situation that can be reduced to a single biochemical step in the cell only in a few cases. Many more parameters in animal experimentation have to be considered than just measurement of the chemistry and physics, often termed “surrogate parameters.” Referring back to the opening quotation, environmental conditions play a crucial role in obtaining reliable scientific results from the models. Overcoming structuralistic views and granting animals a body–mind relation too, probably very similar to humans, does not facilitate animal experimentation and its interpretation. The human–animal boundary is closer.

But besides ethical and moral concerns in general, there are good scientific and economical reasons to scrutinize and carefully optimize laboratory experimentation with animals. Those experiments are costly, need special infrastructure, lots of paperwork and hence quite a number of laboratory staff; each outcome of the experiments should contribute to our knowledge. The battle between hypothesis-driven or explorative research can already be found in the musings of Claude Bernard.2 The demand for both is to extract the maximum of information. Scanning current scientific papers seems, provocatively, to be rather the exception than the rule as the editors and their distinguished invited authors of the present volume show in their contributions. Good laboratory practice as randomization, clear endpoints, sound statistics, selective reporting, and publication bias are at stake and are often ruthlessly abandoned. There seems to be much room for improvement.

Emerging from a fellowship at the Collegium Helveticum, Marianne Martic-Kehl and P. August Schubiger, with their background of active researchers in life sciences, focused for several years on getting hard data about the practice of animal experiments, mostly in rodents, with importance placed on cancer research. Over the years they have continuously confronted their colleagues with their findings and elicited fierce debates in the interdisciplinary environment of the Collegium. The project culminated in a final symposium, the results of which yielded the contents of this book. The author is extremely grateful to both of the editors to have picked up a kind of taboo topic in biomedical research and sometimes stubbornly to follow its traces in the vast universe of biomedical publications.

The book deserves a wide readership the scientific community and beyond.

Gerd Folkers

January 2016

References

  1. 1. Baumans, V. (2004) Use of animals in experimental research: an ethical dilemma? Gene Ther., 11, 64–66.
  2. 2. Soccio, D.J. (2009) Archetypes of Wisdom. 9th edn, Boston, MA: Cengage Learning, p. 55f.
  3. 3. Bentham, J. (1828) Of the limits of the penal branch of jurisprudence. In: An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. A new edition, corrected by the author. Footnote 122: Interest of the inferior animals improperly neglected in legislation, London: Dover Classics, pp. 235, 236.
  4. 4. LaFollette H. and Shanks, N. 1994) Animal experimentation: the legacy of Claude Bernard. Int. Stud. Philos. Sci., 8, 195–210.
  5. 5. Chan, A.W.S. (2013) Progress and prospects for genetic modification of nonhuman primate models in biomedical research. ILAR J., 54, 211–223.