Image

Contents

Preface

Acknowledgments

Abbreviations

Remarks on Transcription

The International Phonetic Alphabet

1 Thinking about Morphology and Morphological Analysis

1.1 What is Morphology?

1.2 Morphemes

1.3 Morphology in Action

1.4 Background and Beliefs

1.5 Introduction to Morphological Analysis

1.6 Summary

2 Words and Lexemes

2.1 What is a Word?

2.2 Empirical Tests for Wordhood

2.3 Types of Words

2.4 Inflection vs. Derivation

2.5 Two Approaches to Morphology: Item-and-Arrangement, Item-and-Process

2.6 The Lexicon

2.7 Summary

3 Morphology and Phonology

3.1 Allomorphs

3.2 Prosodic Morphology

3.3 Primary and Secondary Affixes

3.4 Linguistic Exaptation, Leveling, and Analogy

3.5 Morphophonology and Secret Languages

3.6 Summary

4 Derivation and the Lexicon

4.1 The Saussurean Sign

4.2 Motivation and Compositionality

4.3 Derivation and Structure

4.4 Summary

5 Derivation and Semantics

5.1 The Polysemy Problem

5.2 The Semantics of Derived Lexemes

5.3 Summary

6 Inflection

6.1 What is Inflection?

6.2 Inflection vs. Derivation

6.3 Inventory of Inflectional Morphology Types

6.4 Syncretism

6.5 Typology

6.6 Summary

Agreement in Kujamaat Jóola

7 Morphology and Syntax

7.1 Morphological vs. Syntactic Inflection

7.2 Structural Constraints on Morphological Inflection

7.3 Inflection and Universal Grammar

7.4 Grammatical Function Change

7.5 Summary

Kujamaat Jóola Verb Morphology

Subject and object marking

Aspect

Negative

Emphasis and subordination

Summary

A Brief Survey of Kujamaat Jóola Syntax

8 Morphological Productivity and the Mental Lexicon

8.1 What is Morphological Productivity?

8.2 Productivity and Structure: Negative Prefixes in English

8.3 Degrees of Productivity

8.4 Salience and Productivity

8.5 Testing Productivity

8.6 The Mental Lexicon, Psycholinguistics, and Neurolinguistics

8.7 Conclusion

Answers to Exercises

Glossary

References

Index

Praise for What is Morphology?

“Aronoff and Fudeman have produced a clear and jargon-free introduction to contemporary morphological theory and practice. The book succeeds particularly in clarifying the empirical content, organizational principles and analytic techniques that distinguish morphology from other areas of linguistics.”

James P. Blevins, University of Cambridge

“This book offers abundant examples of morphological data and illuminating guidance through the classic and fundamental problems of morphological analysis. Any student who has worked through this book will really know what morphologists do, and how they go about doing it . . . I consider What Is Morphology? an indispensable introduction to the subject.”

Martin Maiden, FBA,
Professor of the Romance Languages Faculty of Linguistics

Praise for Previous Edition

“Aronoff and Fudeman have provided an extremely pleasant tour of the issues in modern morphological theory for beginning students. The rich collection of exercises will be a godsend to instructors and students alike, and the thread of discussion of a single language throughout the book is a brilliant stroke that other texts should emulate.”

Stephen R. Anderson, Yale University

“This unusual book combines a basic start on morphology with an introduction to Kujamaat Jóola. It is a fine addition to teaching materials on morphology: a book for beginners to use with a teacher, yet one from which any linguist could learn. The authors intend students to develop ‘a lasting taste for morphology’. I think many will.”

Greville Corbett, University of Surrey, Guildford

“Morphology has its own organizing principles, distinct from those of syntax, phonology, and the lexicon. Too many morphology textbooks obscure this fascinating fact, but Aronoff and Fudeman refreshingly make it the cornerstone of their exposition.”

Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy, University of Canterbury

Fundamentals of Linguistics

Each book in the Fundamentals of Linguistics series is a concise and critical introduction to the major issues in a subfield of linguistics, including morphology, semantics, and syntax. The books presuppose little knowledge of linguistics, are authored by well-known scholars, and are useful for beginning students, specialists in other subfields of linguistics, and interested non-linguists.

What is Morphology?

Mark Aronoff and Kirsten Fudeman

What is Meaning? Fundamentals of Formal Semantics

Paul H. Portner

The Fundamentals of Psycholinguistics

Eva M. Fernández and Helen Smith Cairns

Image

Preface

This little book is meant to introduce fundamental aspects of morphology to students with only a minimal background in linguistics. It presupposes only the very basic knowledge of phonetics, phonology, syntax, and semantics that an introductory course in linguistics provides. If, having worked through this book, a student has some understanding of the range of basic issues in morphological description and analysis; can appreciate what a good morphological description looks like, how a good morphological analysis works and what a good theory of morphology does; can actually do morphological analysis at an intermediate level; and most importantly understands that linguistic morphology can be rewarding; then the basic goal of the book will have been met.

The book departs from a trend common among current linguistics textbooks, even at the elementary level, which tend to be quite theoretical in orientation and even devoted to a single theory or set of related theories. We have chosen instead to concentrate on description, analysis, and the fundamental issues that face all theories of morphology. At the most basic level, we want to provide students with a grasp of how linguists think about and analyze the internal structure of complex words in a representative range of real languages. What are the fundamental problems, regardless of one's theoretical perspective? We therefore dwell for the most part on questions that have occupied morphologists since the beginnings of modern linguistics in the late nineteenth century, rather than on more detailed technical points of particular theories.

Of course, this means that we assume that there are general questions, but in morphology, at least, the early modern masters were grappling with many of the same questions that occupy us to this day. Descriptions and analyses that Baudouin de Courtenay wrote in the 1880s are not merely understandable, but even interesting and enlightening to the modern morphologist. The same is true of the work of Edward Sapir and Roman Jakobson from the 1920s and 1930s. Yes, the terminology and theories are different, but the overall goals are much the same. That is not to say that no progress has been made, only that the basic issues about word- internal structure have remained stable for quite a long time.

One fundamental assumption that goes back to the beginnings of modern linguistics is that each language is a system where everything holds together (“la langue forme un système où tout se tient et a un plan d'une merveilleuse rigueur”: Antoine Meillet). More recent linguists have stressed the importance of universal properties that all languages have in common over properties of individual languages, but not even the most radical universalists will deny the systematicity of individual human languages. It is therefore important, from the very beginning, that a student be presented, not just with fragmentary bits of data from many languages, as tends to happen with both morphology and phonology, but with something approaching the entire morphological system of a single language. To that end, we have divided each of the chapters of this book up into two parts. The first part is the conventional sort of material that one would find in any textbook. Here our focus is often on standard American English, although we present data from many other languages, as well. The second part describes in some detail part of the morphology of Kujamaat Jóola, a language spoken in Senegal. For each chapter, we have tried to select an aspect of Kujamaat Jóola morphology that is close to the topic of the chapter. By the end of the book, the student should have a reasonable grasp of the entire system of Kujamaat Jóola morphology and thus understand how, at least for one language, the whole of the morphology holds together. Of course, no one language can be representative of all the world's languages, and morphology is so varied that not even the most experienced analyst is ever completely prepared for what a new language may bring. But students certainly will benefit from a reasonably complete picture of how a single language works.

The Kujamaat Jóola material complements the material in the main portion of the chapter, but it is not meant to mirror it exactly. Our inclusion of particular Kujamaat Jóola topics was dictated in part by the data that were available to us. Our primary sources were J. David Sapir's A Grammar of Diola-Fogny, his 1967 revisions to the analysis of the Kujamaat Jóola verb (Thomas and Sapir 1967), and his unpublished dictionary. In a number of cases, we have used the Kujamaat Jóola section of each chapter to delve into topics not treated in the main portion, or treated only superficially. Thus chapters 2 and 7 contain detailed examinations of Kujamaat Jóola noun classes and verb morphology, respectively, and in chapter 3 we address its rich interactions between vowel harmony and morphology.

We chose Kujamaat Jóola for this book because its morphology, though complex and sometimes unusual, is highly regular, which makes it an excellent teaching vehicle. Some might question this choice, preferring a language with a higher degree of morphological fusion. Such a language might have led to theoretical issues, for example, that we do not explore in any detail here. However, we felt that in a book of this type, aimed at the beginning or intermediate-level morphologist, Kujamaat Jóola was an ideal choice.

One value of presenting beginning students with the largely complete morphological description of a single language is that descriptive grammars (which more often than not concentrate on morphology and phonology) form a mainstay of linguistic research, not only at more advanced levels of study, but throughout a researcher's career. The ability to work through a descriptive grammar is not innate, as many of us assume, but an acquired skill that takes practice. The Kujamaat Jóola sections taken together comprise an almost complete descriptive morphology of that language, so that by the end of the book students will have had the experience of working through an elementary morphological description of one language and will be somewhat prepared to tackle more complete descriptions when the time comes.

This brings us to the topic of how we intend the Kujamaat Jóola sections of this book to be used. Because of their inherent complexity, it is crucial that the instructor not simply assign these sections as readings. Instead, each must be gone over carefully in class until the students have a good grasp of the material in it. Otherwise, students are not likely to extract full value from the Kujamaat Jóola sections. Although we feel that these sections will be both useful and rewarding, it is also the case that the main portions of the chapter are freestanding, and an instructor who prefers not to do some or all of the Kujamaat Jóola sections does not have to.

Each chapter closes with a set of problems that are cross-referenced with the text, and we expect that the solutions to these problems will be discussed in detail in class. Some simpler exercises are integrated into the text itself, with answers provided. We feel that some exercises, particularly open-ended questions, are especially well suited to class discussion, and so instructors may decide not to assign them in written form. Most chapters also contain Kujamaat Jóola exercises designed to get students to apply the data we have provided creatively and analytically. Chapter 1 contains two sample problem sets with answers (section 1.5.3). We suggest that instructors assign these separately from the rest of the chapter reading and that they ask students to write them out as they would a regular assignment, without reading the explanation and analysis that go with them. Then students can check their work on their own. This should prepare them for doing some of the other analytical problems in the text.

Another feature of this book is a glossary. The terms in it appear in bold the first time they are used or explained in the text.

New to the second edition are suggestions for further reading at the end of each chapter. Some of these suggestions are classic treatments of morphological problems, and others represent more recent analyses. We have chosen a number of them because of the clear way in which they illustrate phenomena raised in the chapter. Finally, some of the suggested readings are short enough that instructors might want to assign them in an introduction to morphology class. Other, longer readings could be assigned in part or used by students as they work on morphological problems on their own, whether independently or as a class assignment. While not listed in the further reading for any of the chapters, another extremely useful reference work for students is Bauer's A Glossary of Morphology (2004).

Ideally, each class session will be divided into three parts, corresponding to the division of the chapters: exposition of new pedagogical material; detailed discussion of Kujamaat Jóola; and discussion of solutions for the homework problems of the day (we assume that problems will be assigned daily and that students’ performance on them will comprise a good part of the basis of their grades in the course).

We close with a warning to both the instructor and the student: this book does not pretend to cover all of morphology, but rather only a number of general topics drawn from the breadth of the field that are of special interest to its authors. We have purposely not gone deeply into the aspects of morphology that interact most with other central areas of linguistics (phonology, semantics, and syntax), because that would require knowledge of these areas that beginning students might not have. Thus there is little discussion of clitics, for example. In this, the second edition, we have added more coverage of exciting new work that uses experimental and computational methods, methods that are bound to be more central in the future, but we encourage instructors to supplement our text with current readings in this cutting- edge field. In closing, please permit us to remind the user that our ambitions in writing this volume are quite modest. We do not expect students who have worked through this book to have a full understanding, but to have developed a lasting taste for morphology that, with luck, will sustain them as it has us.

We owe a debt of thanks to the many people who helped us as we worked on this project. We are especially grateful to the various people who read drafts of the manuscript and made suggestions on how to make it better. These include Harald Baayen, Donald Lenfest, Lanko Marusic, and two anonymous Blackwell reviewers. We give special thanks to Phil Baldi and Barbara Bullock, who tested the original manuscript in a morphology class at the Pennsylvania State University, and to five anonymous student reviewers. Their comments were particularly thorough and helped us to improve this book on many different levels. Harald Baayen and some of our anonymous student reviewers also suggested a number of excellent exercises, which we incorporated into the current version. Peter Aronoff read the original manuscript over his winter break and still took a linguistics course the next semester. For their input and discussion, we thank Bill Ham, Alan Nussbaum, and Draga Zec. We are also grateful to Jane Kaplan, who shared her collection of language-related cartoon strips, advertisements, and other magazine and newspaper clippings with us.

J. David Sapir generously gave us permission to reproduce copious amounts of Kujamaat Jóola data from his published and unpublished work, and Eugene Nida allowed us to include exercises first published in his classic textbook on morphology. We are pleased that his exercises will be introduced to a new generation of students.

We are also grateful to the many people who wrote to us after using the first edition of this textbook. Many of them requested an answer key. The second edition indeed has one, available on the Wiley website at www.wiley.com/go/Aronoff. Jenny Mittelstaedt carefully prepared a list of questions and comments that enabled us to make a number of corrections and clarifications to the material presented here. Bill Ham also offered useful suggestions. Finally, reviews of the first edition in print and online by Barli Bram, Malcolm Finney, Margaret Sharp, John Stonham, Gregory Stump, and Jonathan White were enormously helpful to us in identifying elements of the book, small and large, that needed to be revised or updated. In addition to the addition of suggestions for future reading and the expansion (and renaming) of chapter 8, “Morphological Productivity and the Mental Lexicon,” this new edition has been thoroughly revised for style and clarity; it has been updated to reflect current research; its glossary and reference list have been expanded; and some exercises have been revised or added.

This book owes a great deal to the guidance and particularly the patience of the editors at Wiley-Blackwell over the years: Philip Carpenter, Sarah Coleman, Danielle Descoteaux, Tami Kaplan, Julia Kirk, Beth Remmes, and Steve Smith. Thanks also to our project manager, Fiona Sewell. Writing this book has been a joint effort, and we would like to emphasize that the order of the authors’ names given on the title page is alphabetical.

Mark Aronoff [fpref_inline_image001]
and Kirsten Fudeman [fpref_inline_image002]

Acknowledgments

The authors and publisher wish to acknowledge the copyright material used in this book:

p. xx: The International Phonetic Alphabet. Courtesy of the International Phonetic Association (Department of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, School of English, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki 54124, GREECE).

p. 14: Digital DNA™ advertisement. Copyright of Motorola, used by permission.

p. 43: Wizard of Id. By permission of John L. Hart FLP, and Creators Syndicate, Inc. Creators Syndicate, 5777 W. Century Blvd., Suite 700, Los Angeles, CA 90045.

pp. 52–3: Dinka data from Torben Andersen, ‘Vowel quality alternation in Dinka verb inflection’, Phonology 10 (1993): 1–42. By permission of Cambridge University Press.

pp. 23–6, 58–67, 96–100, 131, 148–51, 182–7 and 219–20: Kujamaat Jóola data. From J. David Sapir, A Grammar of Diola-Fogny (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1965). By permission of Cambridge University Press.

p. 98–100: Big–thin distinction in Kujamaat Jóola. From J. David Sapir, ‘Big and thin: two Diola-Fogny meta-linguistic terms’, Language in Society (1975) 4: 1–15. By permission of Cambridge University Press.

The publisher apologizes for any errors or omissions in the above list and would be grateful if notified of any corrections that should be incorporated in future reprints or editions of this book.

Abbreviations

A, adj adjective
abs absolutive
acc accusative
act active
adv adverb
agr agreement
an animate
apass antipassive
app applicative
Ar. Arabic
asp aspect
C consonant
caus causative
cl noun class
ct combining with a circumstantial topic
d declarative
def definite
dem demonstrative
dim diminutive
dir directional
du dual
emph emphatic
erg ergative
excl exclusive
f feminine
foc focus
Fr. French
fut future
fv final vowel
gen genitive
hab habitual
imp imperfective
imper imperative
inan inanimate
inc dubitive-incompletive
incl inclusive
ind indicative
inf infinitive
irr irrealis
loc locative
m masculine
Mdk. Mandinka
n, N noun
ne noun emphasis
neg negative
nom nominative
nonfut non-future
nonhum non-human
NP noun phrase
nts combining with a non-topical subject
obj object
part participle
partic particulizer
pass passive
perf perfective
pl plural
Port. Portuguese
poss possessive
pres present
prog progressive
prtc particle
ps past subordinate
qm question marker
redup reduplicative
refl reflexive
rel relativizer
res resultative
sg singular
stat stative
sub subject
subord subordinating morph
tns tense
tri trial
v, V verb; vowel; theme vowel
VP verb phrase

Remarks on Transcription

Modern linguistics has been struggling with the problem of phonetic and phonological transcription since its inception. The International Phonetics Association was founded in 1886 with the goal of providing for linguistics a worldwide standard system for naming sounds, the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), akin to that universal standard language used in chemistry and physics since the mid- nineteenth century to name the elements and their compounds. But linguists have long resisted this standardization, especially for phonological transcription, much to the dismay of students over the generations. There are many reasons for this resistance. The phonological transcription of a language is often driven by the desire to develop a practical orthography, in which phonetic accuracy and consistency take a back seat to ease of use. Also, phonological theorists since the beginning of that field have enjoyed a love–hate relationship with phonetics, arguing over the true nature of the connection between a phoneme and its various phonetic realizations, leading them to downplay the importance of consistency for phonological transcription across languages, since each language has its own unique phonological system. Leonard Bloomfield, for example, one of the great linguists of the twentieth century, used the symbol U for schwa (IPA inline_image003) in his Menomini grammar, largely for typographical convenience.

In this book, we have made a compromise. Wherever possible or practicable, we have used the IPA, a copy of which is included facing p. 1. We have deviated from the IPA chiefly in our representation of the English approximant rhotic, choosing to use instead the symbol <r> for simplicity. (For more on the International Phonetics Association and the International Phonetic Alphabet, visit the website of the Association at http://www.langsci.ucl.ac.uk/ipa/index.html.) But many languages have well-established orthographies or systems of phonological transcription, which we have not disturbed. Most prominently, in transcribing Kujamaat Jóola, we have adopted wholesale the system used by J. David Sapir in the grammar from which our data and description are adapted. We have endeavored, though, in all cases where transcription departs from the IPA, to give the IPA equivalent for nonstandard symbols.

This lack of consistency may be a little confusing for the student at first, but we hope that it will teach students to be careful, because the symbols used in phonological transcription may sometimes be used in arbitrary and even capricious ways, so that it is important to pay close attention to the phonetic description that accompanies the symbols at their introduction. Reading Bloomfield’s Menomini grammar without knowing that U stands for schwa can lead to serious misunderstanding.

The International Phonetic Alphabet

Revised to 2005

unnumber_figure.001